Acta Psychologica Sinica ›› 2022, Vol. 54 ›› Issue (1): 54-65.doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2022.00054
• Reports of Empirical Studies • Previous Articles Next Articles
YIN Keli(), LAN Miaosen, LI Hui, ZHAO Ziwen
Received:
2021-01-05
Published:
2022-01-25
Online:
2021-11-26
Contact:
YIN Keli
E-mail:yayasles@163.com
Supported by:
YIN Keli, LAN Miaosen, LI Hui, ZHAO Ziwen. (2022). How ritual actions, symbolic meanings, and positive emotions enhance perceived control: A dual path way mechanism. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 54(1), 54-65.
Add to citation manager EndNote|Ris|BibTeX
URL: https://journal.psych.ac.cn/acps/EN/10.3724/SP.J.1041.2022.00054
Picture type | Degree of pleasure | Degree of arousal | Degree of dominance |
---|---|---|---|
Household Goods | 4.57(1.60) | 4.45(1.78) | 5.29(2.20) |
vehicles | 4.55(1.65) | 4.53(1.77) | 5.05(1.92) |
Table 1 Mean (standard deviation)values of pleasure, arousal, and dominance for neutral pictures
Picture type | Degree of pleasure | Degree of arousal | Degree of dominance |
---|---|---|---|
Household Goods | 4.57(1.60) | 4.45(1.78) | 5.29(2.20) |
vehicles | 4.55(1.65) | 4.53(1.77) | 5.05(1.92) |
Ritual action | Symbolic meaning | Positive emotions | Perceived control |
---|---|---|---|
Execute action | gratitude | 10.87 (2.78) | 12.16 (2.15) |
blessing | 12.85 (1.99) | 13.33 (1.85) | |
meaningless expression | 9.94 (2.50) | 12.34 (2.54) | |
No action | gratitude | 10.32 (3.00) | 11.94 (2.62) |
blessing | 12.33 (1.91) | 12.79 (1.88) | |
meaningless expression | 9.72 (2.94) | 11.78 (2.12) |
Table 2 Mean score (standard deviation) of positive emotion and perceived control
Ritual action | Symbolic meaning | Positive emotions | Perceived control |
---|---|---|---|
Execute action | gratitude | 10.87 (2.78) | 12.16 (2.15) |
blessing | 12.85 (1.99) | 13.33 (1.85) | |
meaningless expression | 9.94 (2.50) | 12.34 (2.54) | |
No action | gratitude | 10.32 (3.00) | 11.94 (2.62) |
blessing | 12.33 (1.91) | 12.79 (1.88) | |
meaningless expression | 9.72 (2.94) | 11.78 (2.12) |
Figure 3. The mediating model of the influence of positive emotion on the perceived control by the symbolic meaning of blessing in the non-ritual action condition. Note. **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001
Variables | M ± SD | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
1 the number of generations which participants recited | 4.70 ± 4.73 | 1 | |||
2. Positive emotions | 14.22 ± 1.42 | 0.204 | 1 | ||
3. Gratitude | 22.97 ± 2.72 | 0.175 | 0.483*** | 1 | |
4. Blessing | 20.10 ± 4.25 | 0.084 | 0.251* | 0.698*** | 1 |
5. Perceived control | 13.51 ± 1.94 | 0.317** | 0.243* | 0.055 | -0.012 |
Table 3 Descriptive statistics and correlation analysis of variables
Variables | M ± SD | 1 | 2 | 3 | 4 |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
1 the number of generations which participants recited | 4.70 ± 4.73 | 1 | |||
2. Positive emotions | 14.22 ± 1.42 | 0.204 | 1 | ||
3. Gratitude | 22.97 ± 2.72 | 0.175 | 0.483*** | 1 | |
4. Blessing | 20.10 ± 4.25 | 0.084 | 0.251* | 0.698*** | 1 |
5. Perceived control | 13.51 ± 1.94 | 0.317** | 0.243* | 0.055 | -0.012 |
Variable | β | t | rpartial |
---|---|---|---|
the number of generations which participants recited | 0.279 | 2.349* | 0.282 |
Positive emotions | 0.187 | 1.570 | 0.193 |
Table 4 The results of regression analysis on predictive factors of perceived control (N = 67)
Variable | β | t | rpartial |
---|---|---|---|
the number of generations which participants recited | 0.279 | 2.349* | 0.282 |
Positive emotions | 0.187 | 1.570 | 0.193 |
Projects | Path | β | SE | 95% confidence interval | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Lower | Upper | ||||
Model 1 | Blessing → positive emotion | -0.17 | 0.05 | -0.484 | 0.217 |
Gratitude → positive emotion | 0.60* | 0.08 | 0.012 | 0.898 | |
Positive Emotion → perceived control | 0.28 | 0.19 | -0.015 | 0.535 | |
Blessing → perceived control | -0.05 | 0.08 | -0.425 | 0.329 | |
Gratitude → perceived control | -0.04 | 0.13 | -0.519 | 0.359 | |
Blessing → positive emotion → perceived control | -0.05 | 0.06 | -0.212 | 0.055 | |
Gratitude → positive emotion → perceived control | 0.17* | 0.12 | 0.004 | 0.506 | |
Model 2 | Gratitude → positive emotion | 0.48* | 0.06 | 0.125 | 0.721 |
Gratitude → positive emotion → perceived control | 0.12* | 0.05 | 0.030 | 0.238 | |
Model 3 | Gratitude → positive emotion | 0.48* | 0.06 | 0.125 | 0.721 |
the number of generations ancestors → perceived control | 0.28*** | 0.05 | 0.159 | 0.394 | |
Gratitude → positive emotion → perceived control | 0.09* | 0.05 | 0.009 | 0.208 |
Table 5 Path coefficient and indirect effect test
Projects | Path | β | SE | 95% confidence interval | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Lower | Upper | ||||
Model 1 | Blessing → positive emotion | -0.17 | 0.05 | -0.484 | 0.217 |
Gratitude → positive emotion | 0.60* | 0.08 | 0.012 | 0.898 | |
Positive Emotion → perceived control | 0.28 | 0.19 | -0.015 | 0.535 | |
Blessing → perceived control | -0.05 | 0.08 | -0.425 | 0.329 | |
Gratitude → perceived control | -0.04 | 0.13 | -0.519 | 0.359 | |
Blessing → positive emotion → perceived control | -0.05 | 0.06 | -0.212 | 0.055 | |
Gratitude → positive emotion → perceived control | 0.17* | 0.12 | 0.004 | 0.506 | |
Model 2 | Gratitude → positive emotion | 0.48* | 0.06 | 0.125 | 0.721 |
Gratitude → positive emotion → perceived control | 0.12* | 0.05 | 0.030 | 0.238 | |
Model 3 | Gratitude → positive emotion | 0.48* | 0.06 | 0.125 | 0.721 |
the number of generations ancestors → perceived control | 0.28*** | 0.05 | 0.159 | 0.394 | |
Gratitude → positive emotion → perceived control | 0.09* | 0.05 | 0.009 | 0.208 |
[1] |
Adler, M. G., & Fagley, N. S. (2005). Appreciation: Individual differences in finding value and meaning as a unique predictor of subjective well-being. Journal of Personality, 73(1), 79-114.
doi: 10.1111/jopy.2005.73.issue-1 URL |
[2] |
Alonso-Ferres M., Imami L., & Slatcher R. B. (2019). Untangling the effects of partner responsiveness on health and well-being: The role of perceived control. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 37(4), 1150-1171.
doi: 10.1177/0265407519884726 URL |
[3] | Boyer, P., & Liénard, P. (2006). Why ritualized behavior? Precaution systems and action parsing in developmental, pathological and cultural rituals. Behavioral & Brain Sciences, 29(6), 595-613. |
[4] |
Brooks A. W., Schroeder J., Risen J. L., Gino F., Galinsky A. D., Norton M. I., & Schweitzer M. E. (2016). Don’t stop believing: Rituals improve performance by decreasing anxiety. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 137, 71-85.
doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2016.07.004 URL |
[5] |
Chun, J. W., & Lee, M. J. (2017). When does individuals’ willingness to speak out increase on social media? Perceived social support and perceived power/control. Computers in Human Behavior, 74, 120-129.
doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2017.04.010 URL |
[6] |
Crespo C., Kielpikowski M., Pryor J., & Jose P. E. (2011). Family rituals in New Zealand families: Links to family cohesion and adolescents' well-being. Journal of Family Psychology, 25(2), 184-193.
doi: 10.1037/a0023113 pmid: 21480698 |
[7] |
DeSteno D., Li Y., Dickens L., & Lerner J. S. (2014). Gratitude: A tool for reducing economic impatience. Psychological Science, 25(6), 1262-1267.
doi: 10.1177/0956797614529979 pmid: 24760144 |
[8] | Douglas, M. (2020). Purity and Dange: An analysis of concept of pollution and taboo (Huang J. B., & Liu B. Y., Lu, C.). Beijing: The Commercial Press. (Original work published 1966) |
[9] | Drewelies J., Schade H., Hülür G., Hoppmann C. A., Ram N., & Gerstorf D. (2018). The more we are in control, the merrier? Partner perceived control and negative affect in the daily lives of older couples. Journals of Gerontology: Series B, 75(2), 338-348. |
[10] |
Faul F., Erdfelder E., Lang A. G., & Buchner A. (2007). G* Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175-191.
doi: 10.3758/BF03193146 URL |
[11] |
Fischer R., Callander R., Reddish P., & Bulbulia J. (2013). How do rituals affect cooperation? Human Nature, 24(2), 115-125.
doi: 10.1007/s12110-013-9167-y URL |
[12] |
Gallagher, T. J., & Lewis, J. M. (2010). Rationalists, fatalists, and the modern superstitious: Test-taking in introductory sociology. Sociological Inquiry, 71(1), 1-12.
doi: 10.1111/soin.2001.71.issue-1 URL |
[13] |
Greenaway K. H., Haslam S. A., Cruwys T., Branscombe N. R., Ysseldyk R., & Heldreth C. (2015). From "we" to "me": Group identification enhances perceived personal control with consequences for health and well-being. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 109(1), 53-74.
doi: 10.1037/pspi0000019 URL |
[14] |
Han J. J., Gershoff A. D., Kirmani A., & Dalton A. (2018). When good things feel closer and bad things feel farther: The role of perceived control on psychological distance perception. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 28(4), 629-643.
doi: 10.1002/jcpy.2018.28.issue-4 URL |
[15] | Hayes, A. F. (2013). Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression- based approach. New York: Guilford. |
[16] |
Hobson N. M., Bonk D., & Inzlicht M. (2017). Rituals decrease the neural response to performance failure. Peerj, 5(4), e3363.
doi: 10.7717/peerj.3363 URL |
[17] | Hobson N. M., Gino F., Norton M. I., & Inzlicht, M. (2016). Novel rituals inculcate intergroup bias: Evidence from the trust game and neurophysiology. Social Science Electronic Publishing. |
[18] |
Hobson N. M., Schroeder J., Risen J. L., Xygalatas D., & Inzlicht M. (2018). The psychology of rituals: An integrative review and process-based framework. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 22(3), 260-284.
doi: 10.1177/1088868317734944 URL |
[19] |
Infurna F. J., Gerstorf D., Ram N., Schupp J., & Wagner G. G. (2011). Long-term antecedents and outcomes of perceived control. Psychology and Aging, 26(3), 559.
doi: 10.1037/a0022890 pmid: 21517184 |
[20] |
Kapitány, R., & Nielsen, M. (2015). Adopting the ritual stance: The role of opacity and context in ritual and everyday actions. Cognition, 145, 13-29.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.08.002 pmid: 26298423 |
[21] | Kapitány, R., & Nielsen, M. (2017). The ritual stance and the precaution system: The role of goal-demotion and opacity in ritual and everyday actions. Religion, Brain & Behavior, 7(1), 27-42. |
[22] |
Legare, C. H., & Souza, A. L. (2018). Evaluating ritual efficacy: Evidence from the supernatural. Cognition, 124(1), 1-15.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.03.004 URL |
[23] |
Legare, C. H., & Souza, A. L. (2014). Searching for control: Priming randomness increases the evaluation of ritual efficacy. Cognitive Science, 38(1), 152-161.
doi: 10.1111/cogs.2014.38.issue-1 URL |
[24] |
Legare C. H., Wen N. J., Herrmann P. A., & Whitehouse H. (2015). Imitative flexibility and the development of cultural learning. Cognition, 142, 351-361.
doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2015.05.020 URL |
[25] | Lin, C. C. (2019). Gratitude, positive emotion, and satisfaction with life: A test of mediated effect. Social Behavior and Personality: An International Journal, 47(4), 1-8. |
[26] | Long, X. T. (2019). The performance and connotation of generational “Death Education” and its modern value. Religious Studies, 4, 269-275. |
[27] | Malinowski, B. (1948). Magic, science and religion and other essays. Boston, MA: Beacon Press. |
[28] |
Martin L., Jan K., Shaver J. H., Danijela J., & Dimitris X. (2015). Effects of anxiety on spontaneous ritualized behavior. Current Biology, 25(14), 1892-1897.
doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.05.049 pmid: 26096971 |
[29] |
Mitkidis P., Ayal S., Shalvi S., Heimann K., Levy G., Kyselo M., … Roepstorff A. (2017). The effects of extreme rituals on moral behavior: The performers-observers gap hypothesis. Journal of Economic Psychology, 59, 1-7.
doi: 10.1016/j.joep.2016.12.007 URL |
[30] | Nielbo, K.L., & Sørensen, J. (2011). Spontaneous processing of functional and non-functional action sequences. Religion, Brain & Behavior, 1(1), 18-30. |
[31] |
Norton, M. I., & Gino, F. (2014). Rituals alleviate grieving for loved ones, lovers, and lotteries. Journal of Experimental Psychology General, 143(1), 266-272.
doi: 10.1037/a0031772 URL |
[32] | Park, C. (1999). The influence of the consumption ritualization on buying behaviors. Estud Gerenc, 30(6), 1340-1348. |
[33] |
Piedmont, R. L. (1999). Does spirituality represent the sixth factor of personality? Spiritual transcendence and the five - factor model. Journal of Personality, 67(6), 985-1013.
doi: 10.1111/jopy.1999.67.issue-6 URL |
[34] | Ping, A. J., & Peng, K. P. (2018). Gratitude facilitates self-control. Studies of Psychology and Behavior, 16(3), 378-383. |
[35] |
Ran Y. X., Wei H. Y., Li Q., & Lei C. (2018). Ritual in the psychology field: A repetitive but meaningful behavior. Advances in Psychological Science, 26(1), 169-179.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2018.00169 URL |
[36] |
Ruiz-Aranda D., Salguero J. M., Cabello R., Palomera R., & Berrocal P. F. (2018). Can an emotional intelligence program improve adolescents' psychosocial adjustment? Results from the INTEMO project. Social Behavior and Personality: An International Journal, 40(8), 1373-1379.
doi: 10.2224/sbp.2012.40.8.1373 URL |
[37] |
Ruthig J. C., Chipperfield J. G., Perry R. P., Newall N. E., & Swift A. (2007). Comparative risk and perceived control: Implications for psychological and physical well-being among older adults. The Journal of Social Psychology, 147(4), 345-369.
doi: 10.3200/SOCP.147.4.345-369 URL |
[38] |
Ruthig J. C., Perry R. P., Hladkyj S., Hall N. C., Pekrun R., & Chipperfield J. G. (2008). Perceived control and emotions: Interactive effects on performance in achievement settings. Social Psychology of Education, 11(2), 161-180.
doi: 10.1007/s11218-007-9040-0 URL |
[39] |
Sebestény, A., & Emmons, N. (2017). Balinese hindus’ afterlife beliefs as stable constructs: An effect of high frequency domestic rituals. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 17(5), 437-462.
doi: 10.1163/15685373-12340013 URL |
[40] | Se, Y. (2012). A religious anthropological analysis about ancestor worship. Journal of Inner Mongolia Normal University (Philosophy & Social Science), 41(3), 29-34. |
[41] | Tian A. D., Schroeder J., Häubl G., Risen J. L., Norton M. I., & Gino F. (2018). Enacting rituals to improve self-control. Journal of Personality & Social Psychology, 114(6), 851-876. |
[42] |
Vohs K. D., Wang Y. J., Gino F., & Norton M. I. (2013). Rituals enhance consumption. Psychological Science, 24(9), 1714-1721.
doi: 10.1177/0956797613478949 URL |
[43] |
Warburton W. A., Williams K. D., & Cairns D. R. (2006). When ostracism leads to aggression: The moderating effects of control deprivation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 42(2), 213-220.
doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2005.03.005 URL |
[44] |
Wood A. M., Joseph S., & Linley P. A. (2007). Coping style as a psychological resource of grateful people. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 26(9), 1076-1093.
doi: 10.1521/jscp.2007.26.9.1076 URL |
[45] | Wu M. L. (2010). Practice of questionnaire statistical analysis (p.244). Chongqing: Chongqing University Press. |
[46] | Xiong Y. X. (2015). Religion research on Pumi of China. China Social Sciences Press. |
[47] |
Xu Q., Kwan C. M., & Zhou X. (2020). Helping yourself before helping others: How perceived control promotes charitable behaviors. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 30(3), 486-505.
doi: 10.1002/jcpy.v30.3 URL |
[48] | Yin, K. L., & He, J. M. (2018). Reliability and validity of the mental health continuum short form in adults. Chinese Mental Health Journal, 26(5), 388-392. |
[49] |
Zeigler-Hill, V. (2006). Contingent self-esteem and the interpersonal circumplex: The interpersonal pursuit of self-esteem. Personality and Individual Differences, 40(4), 713-723.
doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2005.08.009 URL |
[50] | Zhang, H. Y. (2000). Pumi's "Guolu" and "Zhong Bala". Folklore Studies, (1), 132-135. |
[51] |
Zheng L., Miao M., & Gan Y. Q. (2020). Perceived control buffers the effects of the COVID-19 pandemic on general health and life satisfaction: The mediating role of psychological distance. Applied Psychology: Health and Well-Being, 12(4), 1095-1114.
doi: 10.1111/aphw.v12.4 URL |
[52] |
Zhou X., He L., Yang Q., Lao J., & Baumeister R. F. (2018). Control deprivation and styles of thinking. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 102(3), 460.
doi: 10.1037/a0026316 URL |
[53] |
Zou X. Y., Yin K. L., & Lu L. (2018). Collective rituals promote cohesion: Based on action, emotion, and memory. Advances in Psychological Science, 26(5), 939-950.
doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2018.00939 URL |
[1] | JIANG Yuan-Ping, JIANG Cheng-Ming, HU Tian-Yi, SUN Hong-Yue. Effects of emotion on intertemporal decision-making: Explanation from the single dimension priority model [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2022, 54(2): 122-140. |
[2] | FENG Biao, XU Liang, ZHANG Weixing, CHEN Ting, WANG Wenqing, ZHENG Xifu. The inhibitive effect of positive emotions on fear generalization [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2017, 49(3): 317-328. |
[3] | YANG Shenlong, GUO Yongyu, HU Xiaoyong, SHU Shouli, LI Jing. Do lower class individuals possess higher levels of system justification? An examination from the social cognitive perspectives [J]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 2016, 48(11): 1467-1478. |
[4] | HE Xiao-Li,WANG Zhen-Hong,WANG Ke-Jing. The Influence of Positive Emotions on Interpersonal Trust: Clues Effects [J]. , 2011, 43(12): 1408-1417. |
[5] | Wang Yanmei,Guo Dejun. The Effects of Positive Emotions on Task Switchin [J]. , 2008, 40(03): 301-306. |
[6] | Wang Dengfeng, Gan Yiqun (Department of Psychology,Beijing University). A COGNITIVE-KNOWLEDGE SYSTEM MODEL OF PERCEIVED CONTROLLABILITY ON LIFE EVENTS [J]. , 1994, 26(01): 69-76. |
Viewed | ||||||
Full text |
|
|||||
Abstract |
|
|||||